183x Filetype PDF File size 0.41 MB Source: scielo.isciii.es
anales de psicología, 2018, vol. 34, nº 2 (may), 368-377 © Copyright 2018: Editum. Servicio de Publicaciones de la Universidad de Murcia. Murcia (Spain) http://dx.doi.org/10.6018/analesps.34.2.286191 ISSN print edition: 0212-9728. ISSN web edition (http://revistas.um.es/analesps): 1695-2294 Positive visual reframing: A randomised controlled trial using drawn visual imagery to defuse the intensity of negative experiences and regulate emotions in healthy adults 1 2* Julia Clare Ruppert *, and Francisco José Eiroa-Orosa 1 Collective Arts (England, UK). 2 Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychobiology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Barcelona (Spain). Título: Reformulación Visual Positiva: Un ensayo controlado aleatorizado Abstract: This research explores the outcome of positive visual reframing utilizando imágenes visuales para disminuir la intensidad de experiencias (PVR), a single session intervention where drawn images of negative expe- negativas y regular emociones en adultos sanos. riences and open memories were redrawn and visually reframed to form Resumen: Se exploran los resultados de la reformulación visual positiva new positive narratives. The study hypothesised that PVR would lead to (RVP), una intervención en la que imágenes de memorias abiertas y expe- improvements to positive and negative affect, self-efficacy and the per- riencias negativas son reformuladas visualmente para formar nuevas narra- ceived intensity and perceived resolution of a selected negative experience. tivas positivas. El estudio plantea la hipótesis de que la RVP conduciría a Healthy adults (n = 62) were randomly assigned to the PVR or control mejoras en el afecto positivo y negativo, autoeficacia e intensidad y resolu- condition. For the experimental group, statistical significance was identi- ción percibidas de experiencias negativas. 62 adultos sanos fueron aleatori- fied for positive affect and the perceived intensity and resolution of the zados a condiciones de RVP o control. Se hallaron mejoras para el grupo negative experience immediately following the PVR activity. Self-efficacy experimental en niveles de afecto positivo, así como intensidad percibida y was marginally significant. The findings highlight the potential of positive resolución de la experiencia negativa inmediatamente después de comple- visual reframing to enhance emotional regulation when negative emotions tar la actividad de RVP. Estos hallazgos ponen de relieve el potencial de la are triggered. At two weeks‟ post-intervention, improvements were identi- RVP para mejorar la regulación emocional cuando se activan emociones fied in both conditions. This suggests that over time, the visual and senso- negativas. ry exposure created by drawing a negative memory may also lead to posi- A las dos semanas se identificaron mejoras en ambas condiciones. Esto su- tive gains. The study emphasises the potential of PVR to regulate emo- giere que, con el tiempo, la exposición viso-sensorial creada al extraer un tions and defuse the intensity of negative or open memories by visually recuerdo negativo también puede conducir a mejoría. transforming a moment of peak perceptual intensity. Future studies ex- El estudio hace hincapié en el potencial de la RVP para regular emociones ploring the effectiveness of positive visual reframing to shift negative emo- y difuminar la intensidad de un recuerdo abierto mediante la transforma- tions in clinical and non-clinical populations are recommended. ción visual de un momento de máxima intensidad perceptual. Se reco- Keywords: Drawn imagery, reframing, negative experiences, open miendan estudios que exploren la efectividad de la RVP para cambiar per- memory, emotional regulation. cepciones negativas en poblaciones clínicas y no clínicas. Palabras clave: Imágenes dibujadas, reformulación, recuerdos negativos, regulación emocional. Introduction more complex and less coherent narratives than closed or positive memories (Bohanek, Fivush, & Walker, 2005), the Negative experiences are something we all share and general- negative uncertainty of open memories further enhances ly arise as a consequence of emotionally charged social inter- negative emotions (Bar-Anan, Wilson, & Gilbert, 2009) and actions and events (John & Gross, 2004). When attempts to perpetuate questioning about „why‟ the experience happened understand and explain the meaning of a negative experience (Abele, 1985). Finally, because open memories are so acces- are successful, we generally find closure and move on (Wil- sible they make substantial contributions to current feelings son, & Gilbert, 2008). Otherwise, the experience remains and decision-making (Beike, & Writh-Beaumont, 2005). As humans are highly motivated to make sense of themselves „open‟ and is easily triggered by reminiscent experiences and and their experiences, these poorly understood negative emotional and visual cues (Lazarus, 1991). Described collo- memories are also perceived as a source of threat (Wilson & quially as „unfinished business‟ (Beike, & Writh-Beaumont, Gilbert, 2008). This means when triggered, open memories 2005), open memories are problematic for a variety of rea- receive priority cognitive attention (Allport & Postman, sons. For one, open memories contain high levels of emo- 1946; Malle & Knobe, 1997) and are automatically selected tional detail, emotional significance, and information, which for processing and appraisal at the expense of working when combined greatly increases the likelihood the memory memory performance (Klein & Boals, 2001; Curci Lanciano, will be maintained (Conway et al., 1994; Peeters, & Cza- Soleti, & Rimé, 2013). pinski, 1990). Because negative memories are perceived as To respond to the demands of a difficult emotion, open self-relevant, they lead to intensified affective reactions upon memories activate a range of emotional-regulation strategies recall (Frijda, 1988) and challenge effective emotional analy- (Ochsner, & Gross, 2005). Cognitive reappraisal, for exam- sis (Metcalfe, & Mischel, 1999). As these memories contain ple, is an adaptive regulation process, whereby reframing an event is used to diminish emotional impact and shift a nega- * Correspondence address [Dirección para correspondencia]: tive trajectory of response (John, & Gross, 2004; Lazarus, & Julia Ruppert. Collective Arts, 6 Beech Way, Twickenham. TW2 5JT, Eng- Alfert, 1964). Demonstrating the benefits of adaptive emo- land (UK). E-mail: info@collective-arts.org tional regulation strategies, frequent use of cognitive reap- Francisco José Eiroa-Orosa. Section of Personality, Evaluation and Psycho- logical Treatment. Faculty of Psychology, University of Barcelona. Passeig praisal is related to higher levels of positive and lower levels Vall d'Hebron, 171, 08035 Barcelona (Spain). E-mail: feiroa@ub.edu - 368 - Positive visual reframing: A randomised controlled trial using drawn visual imagery to defuse the intensity of negative experiences and regulate emotions in healthy adults 369 of negative affect and is positively related to mood repair. quality of visual interventions to bolster the cognitive re- Habitual reappraisers also display high self-esteem, high op- sources open memories drain. Finally, whereas meaningful timism and fewer depressive tendencies (John, & Gross, and relevant visual cues can unconsciously activate negative 2004). Like cognitive reappraisal, positive reframing is a emotional responses (Öhman, & Mineka, 2001; Folkman, regulatory process of perceiving an experience or event pre- Lazarus, Dunkel-Schetter, DeLongis, & Gruen, 1986), visual viously regarded as negative within a new context or frame- cues can trigger positive emotional responses as well (Latu, work (Lambert et al., 2009). Although both cognitive reap- Mast, Lammers, & Bombardi, 2013; Winkielman, Berridge, praisal and positive reframing are generally understood as & Wilbarger, 2005). outcomes of disposition and personality traits (Packenham, In related evidence, the effectiveness of venting negative & Cox, 2008; Lambert et al., 2009), researchers argue there is experiences through drawing and writing is investigated in promised to be had by interventions that can highlight func- several empirical studies. Chan and Horneffer (2006) (n = tional emotional regulation strategies (Ehring, Tuschen- 63) for example, found that in the short term, drawing mean- Caffier, Schnulle, et al., 2010). ingful negative experiences elicited stronger negative emo- Whereas positively reinterpreting negative experiences tions and was less effective in decreasing social dysfunction provides an adaptive response that boosts coping resources than writing about them. Contrasting findings by Pizarro (Lyubomirsky, Sheldon, & Schkade 2005), intervention stud- (2004) (n = 45), which show in the short-term writing about ies exploring positive reframing of open memories have had trauma leads to higher levels of negative affect than drawing, limited empirical attention. Instead, research on open suggests research is inconclusive in this area. However, when memory closure has largely focused on emotional venting the valence of drawn imagery is shifted from negative to pos- through expressive writing paradigms (Pennebaker, 1993). itive or benign, interesting shifts arise. Using positive-themed An important exception to this somewhat negatively focused drawn imagery to distract attention away from a negatively strategy is Watkins, Cruz, Holben, and Kolts (2008) who induced mood was found more effective than using drawn demonstrate the positive impact of grateful written pro- imagery to vent negative feelings (Dalebroux, Goldstein, & cessing on open memory closure. Winner, 2008; Drake & Winner, 2012). This suggests posi- Despite a research emphasis on expressive writing for tive-focused drawing may have benefits over negative- negative memory closure, it is important to note that memo- focused drawing. However, neither venting nor positive dis- ries and experiences are generally summarised as visual imag- traction was sufficiently effective, in the short term, to re- es (Conway, 2009; Holmes & Mathews, 2010) and that nega- duce the negative arousal created by either an induced nega- tive and traumatic memories are encoded as sensory percep- tive mood (De Petrillio & Winner, 2005) or an authentic tions (van der Kolk & van der Hart, 1995). Subsequently, as open memory (Dalebroux et al., 2008). Subsequently, defus- emotional systems both respond to and store information us- ing negative arousal may require a more meaningful strategy ing sensory-based formats, these systems may actually be less than venting or positive distraction currently provide. Finally, responsive to non-sensory based interventions that use higher it is plausible that like expressive writing, negative arousal in order level conscious processing such as language and writ- the short-term is a necessary requirement of longer-term ing (Holmes & Mathews, 2010). Based on this conclusion, as positive gain (Pennebaker, Kiecolt-Glaser, & Glaser, 1988). negative open memories arise from and are recorded as visu- Empirical studies exploring the use of drawn visual im- al experiences, they might actually require sensory-based, (ie. agery to tackle negative experiences are limited but include visual) interventions to shift deeply rooted emotional re- Mercer, Warson, and Zhao (2010), who used a single group sponses and perceptions (Holmes, Arntz, & Smucker, 2007; (N=10), test-re-test design and a range of intervention tech- Mathews, Ridgeway, & Holmes, 2013). Furthermore, as im- niques to explore the impact of drawing benign versions of age-based memories are considered fragile (Beike & Writh- work-related stress. Findings showed that while positive af- Beaumont, 2005), malleable (Conway, 2009; Holmes & fect remained unchanged, anxiety and negative affect de- Mathews, 2010) and progressively reconstructed over time creased. In contrast, Holmes and Mathews (2010) found that (Torralba, & Oliva, 2003), drawn visuals might provide an imagining benign, ambiguous outcomes to a negative experi- especially synergic approach for reframing and supporting ence was ineffective at decreasing anxiety, yet was later rem- memory closure. edied when participants imagined unambiguously positive Additional support for the viability of visual interven- resolutions. In a more recent study, healthcare professionals tions comes from studies that show art-making enhances (n = 35) took part in a multi-levelled intervention using positive mood (Bell, & Robbins, 2007; De Petrillo & Winner, drawn imagery to de-stress (Huss & Sarid, 2014). Measures 2005). A positive mood, in turn, leads to increased levels of indicated that between pre and post-intervention, subjective mental flexibility and creative thinking (Forgas, 1998; Isen, levels of discomfort were halved. The authors argue their 2002), facilitates cognitive processing (Taylor, 1991) and finding lends support to links between the transformation of primes for cognitive change (Kuvaasa, & Selart, 2004). imagery and the transformation of subjective states. They Therefore, given the tendency of open memories to deplete further propose that image-transformation may lead to per- cognitive resources (Klein, & Boals, 2001; Curci et al., 2013), ceived control and provide a trigger for a more relaxed emo- there is an additional argument for the mood-enhancing tional state. Although not specifically highlighted, like Mercer anales de psicología, 2018, vol. 34, nº 2 (may) 370 Julia Clare Ruppert, and Francisco José Eiroa-Orosa et al. (2010) it is also plausible that as based on peak and end Method theory (Fredrickson, 2000), visually defusing a moment of peak intensity led to new, less negative perceptions of the Design experience‟s ending. While each of these studies has their limitations (lack of control group or randomisation, small Due to a shortfall of empirical studies on drawn image- sample sizes, or the challenge of extracting key impacts from based interventions, the present research was designed as a a multi-intervention focus), for the current research they randomised control trial to explore between-group and with- provide an important springboard for exploration. At pre- in-group outcomes. The research was developed as a practi- sent, drawn image-based interventions are an underexplored cal and deliverable intervention through which the impact of area of quantitative investigation, particularly within non- a single independent variable (positive visual reframing) clinical populations and currently lack representation in in- could be assessed as a stand-alone activity. The dependent tervention-led fields such as positive psychology. variables, positive and negative affect and self-efficacy were To address this gap in the literature, the present study selected as clear indicators of emotional regulation and posi- explored the impact of Positive Visual Reframing (PVR) – de- tive psychological gain. Ordinal scales measuring the intensi- scribed here as a functional emotional regulation strategy ty and resolution of the negative experience were included to that uses drawn visual imagery to positively reframe and de- add a richer nuance to the findings of the intervention and fuse negative emotional experiences. The study hypothesised provide clear data on perceptual shifts. Demographics, in- that after producing an image of an open memory or nega- cluding the age of the negative experience, were also includ- tive emotional experience, healthy adults who used drawn ed in the design. visual imagery to positively reframe their memory‟s narrative would have higher scores on positive affect, self-efficacy and Participants perceived resolution and lower scores on negative affect and the perceived intensity of the experience than a control Participants were healthy adult volunteers (n = 62) of group who undertook a neutral drawing activity. working age living in the UK. A total of 46 females and 14 males took part in the study. Two participants did not identi- fy their gender. See Consort Flow Diagram (fig. 1) and base- line characteristics (table 1). Figure 1. Flow diagram of the study. anales de psicología, 2018, vol. 34, nº 2 (may) Positive visual reframing: A randomised controlled trial using drawn visual imagery to defuse the intensity of negative experiences and regulate emotions in healthy adults 371 Over a three-month time-frame, participants were re- (very strong positive effect). Participants circled one re- cruited widely by the principal investigator through Face- sponse to describe the current intensity of their selected ex- book, LinkedIn shares, and call-outs for volunteers in vari- perience or challenge. Perceived resolution of the negative ous organisational, educational and vocational newsletters. A experience was assessed using a 7-point Likert scale. Re- sample size of 60+ participants was selected as, accepting an sponses ranged from -3 (very unresolved) to +3 (very re- alpha risk of 0.05 and a beta risk of 0.2 in a two-sided test. solved) based on the question, „How resolved or unresolved would At least 29 subjects were needed to recognise as statistically you currently rate your feelings toward this memory or experience?’ significant a difference greater than or equal to 0.5 standard deviations, assuming a correlation coefficient between the in- Procedure itial and final measurement of r = 0.8. All participants re- ceived an introductory letter and a consent form prior to tak- Following written instructions, study participants com- ing part in the study. pleted Time-1 baseline, measures for positive and negative A copy of a sealed, coded and shuffled intervention pack, affect using the PANAS Scale (Watson & Clark, 1988), men- randomly allocating subjects to the experimental or control tal well-being using the Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well- condition was distributed to participants. Each pack con- being Scale (WEMWB) (Tennant et al., 2007), and self- tained written instructions, blank white A4 paper, study efficacy using the General Self-Efficacy Scale (GSE) measures for Time-1-3 and Time-4 (two weeks‟ post- (Schwarzer & Jerusalem, 1995). Next, using a pencil and intervention) and a postage-paid return envelope for return- blank paper, participants in both conditions created an im- ing the data. The two-week follow-up time frame was select- age, doodle or diagram of a negative experience, open ed as representative of when short-term intervention benefits memory or challenge. After completing and reflecting on the generally wane (Semmer, 2008). The study was double- negative image, all participants completed Time-2 measures blinded as neither the researcher nor the participants knew for positive and negative affect, self-efficacy, ordinal scales which packs participants would receive. The design of the on the perceived intensity and perceived resolution of their control and experimental activities aimed to encourage both image‟s memory and details of the experience‟s age. groups to view their activity as the main focus of the study. At Time-3, the experimental group used positive visual To counter the influence of participant bias or priming, par- reframing to positively change the narrative of their image by ticipants worked independently and self-selected a time and drawing over the initial pencil drawing with a black pen or location to complete the study. felt-tip and making note of and including any fresh insight in their new image. Once complete, participants used a rubber Measures to remove any unwanted traces of the original negative pen- cil drawing, re-told themselves the new positive story in their The healthiness of subsects to take part in the study was mind and took a few moments to connect with any positive assessed using Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale feelings created. The same measures used at Time-2 were (WEMWB) (Tennant et al., 2007). The WEMWB is a vali- then completed for Time-3. In the control group, partici- dated, ordinal scale measuring mental health across 14 de- pants covered a fresh sheet of blank A4 paper with triangles scriptive phrases. For the purposes of this study, well-being as a neutral, image-based activity, and then went on to com- was defined as “the balance between an individual‟s resource plete the Time-3 repeat-measures. pool and the challenges faced” (p. 230, Dodge, Daley, Huy- After completing the intervention, all participants were ton, & Saunders, 2012). instructed to take a photograph of their final image and set it Positive and negative affect were measured using the as their mobile phone‟s background for a period of two Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) (Watson, & weeks. This activity was selected as positive replay maintains Clark, 1988). Positive affect is described as a high state of positive emotions (Lyubomirsky, Sousa & Dickerhoof, concentration, energy, and pleasurable engagement while 2006). Intervention packs were returned in postage-paid en- negative affect is described an aversive mood state of un- velopes minus the drawings. These were purposefully ex- pleasurable engagement and distress (Watson, & Clark, cluded from the study to encourage participant willingness to 1998). create meaningful personal narratives and pre-empt any anx- Self-efficacy was measured using the Generalized Self- ieties that drawings might be interpreted, or artistry judged. Efficacy Scale (GSE) (Schwarzer & Jerusalem, 1995). Self- Two weeks after completing the intervention, written in- efficacy is concerned with an individual‟s perceptual belief in structions requested participants in all conditions to mentally their own capabilities and influences how people think, feel recall the original negative experience before completing and and behave (Bandura, 1977). returning via post a final set of Time-4 repeat-measures. Up- Ordinal scales assessing perceived competence and per- on receipt of the measures, participants received a study- ceived resolution were based on Beike and Writh-Beaumont debriefing sheet including a copy of the PVR instructions for (2005) and Watkins et al. (2008). Perceived intensity of the control group volunteers. negative experience was assessed using a 9-point Likert scale. Responses ranged from 1 (very strong negative effect) to 9 anales de psicología, 2018, vol. 34, nº 2 (may)
no reviews yet
Please Login to review.